Issue:
May-June, 1999 © Skeptical Inquirer
Recent analyses propose
that the 1967 film of "Bigfoot" documents a large, feral nonhuman
primate unknown to modern science. Known sources of measurement error
and existing data on human locomotion suggest a more cautious conclusion.Of the varied sources
of evidence invoked to support the existence of Bigfoot, none is more
widely cited than the 1967 film of a large, hairy bipedal figure walking
along the Bluff Creek drainage in northern California. Known as the Patterson-Gimlin
film, this short motion picture (less than one minute running time) has
generated considerable controversy with respect to its authenticity. The
film depicts a burly figure walking deliberately away from the cameraman.
The footage is often blurry due to excessive camera movement. In the most
famous frame, the film subject turns its head and shoulders toward the
camera to peer at its pursuer. Details of the subject's physiognomy cannot
be discerned.Recently, three independent
studies have presented arguments suggesting that fabrication of the film
would have been unlikely or impossible given the technology of the day.
The implication, therefore, is that giant, bipedal primates have inhabited
wilderness areas of the Pacific Northwest in recent times, yet have remained
undiscovered by wildlife biologists and unrecognized among the mainstream
scientific community.In separate studies,
Chris Murphy of Progressive Research(1) and Jeff Glickman of Photek and
the North American Science Institute (NASI),(2) achieved a remarkable
convergence of results when both investigators concluded, via different
methods of estimation, that the film subject stood fully 7 [feet] 3 1/2
[inches] (222 cm) tall. Glickman's study (1998) also concluded that the
chest of the figure measured a hefty 83 [inches] in circumference and
that the film subject weighed in at 1,957 pounds.In his book Big Footprints
(1992), Grover Krantz makes two claims with respect to the film. First,
Krantz argues that no human exists whose body dimensions match those of
the film subject, even given the effects of a furry costume. Second, he
claims that the kinematics of the film subject are decidedly nonhuman,
that the gait could not be duplicated by a person wearing a costume.In essence, two claims
seriously undermine the hypothesis that the Patterson-Gimlin film is a
hoax: (1) that the film subject's body dimensions are outside the range
of human variation, and (2) that the gait of the film subject cannot be
duplicated by a person.In this report, we
argue that the exact dimensions of the film subject are unknowable and
that the gait of the film subject is easily reproducible by human beings
of average stature. Neither of these arguments demonstrate that the subject
of the film is not a Bigfoot; we simply wish to point out that recent
trumpetings in cyberspace about the film's authenticity may not enjoy
a solid empirical foundation.(3)
Analyzing the Claims
It is alleged that the film subject left tracks on the Bluff Creek sandbar,
which were cast subsequent to filming. The tracks measured 14 1/2[inches]
in length. About ten days after the film was made, Bigfoot investigator
Bob Titmus reconstructed the subject's course of travel during filming
and attempted to establish the position of Roger Patterson's camera during
the event. These reconstructions were performed without a measuring tape
or camera(4); quantitative efforts to map the positions of the cameraman
and film the subject were made in subsequent years.(5) Thus, we know the
general course of travel of the film subject but not its exact traverse
(cf. Byrne 1975, Green 1981).
Murphy assesses the subject's height by taking a known quantity - subject
heel width from footprint casts - and using this scalar as a calibration
standard to determine film subject dimensions. Given the heel calibration
and a "stoop factor" correction, he arrives at the stature of
7[feet]3 1/2[inches]. Murphy suggests that a stick recovered from the
film site years after the event is also an appropriate calibration standard
because the subject ostensibly is seen stepping over this stick during
the film. Using this independent criterion, he obtains the same result.
In the NASI report, Glickman employs a third method that also yields the
same height for the film subject. His method involves using a later photograph
from the film site of an individual of known height, purportedly standing
along the original path of the film subject, as a basis from which to
determine the film subject's dimensions. This calibration photograph was
taken by long-time Bigfoot investigator Peter Byrne five years after the
film was made, and Glickman uses the alignment of dead trees that appear
in the background of the film to match the photo with the relevant film
frames.
Murphy and Glickman recognize that for a calibration standard to be valid,
the object used for calibration must be coplanar with the film subject,(6)
such that the calibration object and subject are equidistant from the
optical axis of the camera. It is also well-established that, in order
to minimize error, a calibration standard must be sufficiently large relative
to the object being measured. Given the uncertainty of subject position
in the film, it is not clear that objects chosen for calibration purposes
lie completely within the intended reference plane. The dimension to be
used in calibration and the subject of interest must be positioned equidistant
to the camera lens to provide accurate measurement. Thus, a scalar dimension
(a known quantity with which to scale other dimensions on an object or
image) measured from a calibration standard that is not exactly coincident
with a reference plane, even if that standard occupies a point in that
plane, will yield uncorrectable errors if this obliquity is present (i.e.,
the standard is not aligned with the reference plane) but its degree is
not known.For the same reason,
measurement error can occur if scalar dimensions and the subject occupy
a desired reference plane but when the camera's optical axis is not positioned
perpendicular to that plane. This problem becomes particularly acute in
the context of the Patterson-Gimlin film because camera position was not
controlled relative to the subject's movements along a path defining the
reference plane; thus, "coplanar" standard and subject may not
be equidistant to the camera lens. Objects that are actually not coplanar
may appear to be so if the camera lens is obliquely set relative to the
true plane of reference. To illustrate the problems posed by these sources
of error, we estimated a human subject's stature from videotape recordings
using calibration objects of known dimension as scalars under ideal laboratory
conditions.
One of us (Daegling) was filmed at a distance of 490 cm from the camera
[ILLUSTRATION FOR FIGURE 1 OMITTED]. Three sources of calibration were
used: a two-meter standard, Daegling's foot length, and his heel width.
These standards were digitized on the image(7) and used to estimate Daegling's
true height (194.5 cm). As expected, the two-meter standard yielded a
very good estimate of stature (193.6 cm). Repeated measures within observers
indicated that digitizing error was negligible, and the between-observer
standard deviation was a respectable 0.44 cm. Using Daegling's foot length
as a standard, the error increases markedly (204.3 cm or 3 percent above
the true value). Using a heel-width calibration standard the error balloons
to 28 percent (249.4 cm, with pronounced inter-observer error [sd = 6.5]).
These figures make no allowance for camera obliquity or objects off the
intended reference plane. If either of these factors are introduced, errors
will increase. For example, when foot length is offset from the reference
plane by 10 degrees but the length of the foot is considered to be the
same as before, stature estimates are off by about 10 percent (214.9 cm).
At a 20-degree offset, the overestimate is about 17 percent (227.7 cm).
The degree to which the foot is out of plane cannot be reliably assessed
from an image unless an independent scalar exists in the plane of reference.
Ostensibly Murphy has another scalar in the stick over which the film
subject steps early in the film. Not only is this calibration object relatively
small, but there is also no way to verify that the long axis of the stick
(the intended scalar dimension) is perpendicular to the camera lens. These
observations suggest that the magnitude of error introduced in Murphy's
method is unknown, is probably unacceptably large, and cannot be corrected
given the known parameters of the film.
Glickman's method is far superior since the calibration standard is relatively
large.(8) is asserted that the film subject's course of travel in Figure
6 of the NASI report and the standard included in the figure (an individual
at the film site) occupy the intended reference plane. The report does
not specify how the coincidence of film subject and standard in this plane
is verified. The calibration standard was scaled by superimposing dead
trees from the background of the 1972 photograph onto the 1967 film. There
may be errors associated with this superimposition, but their magnitude
is not known. In any case, this alignment, however precise, does not establish
that the standard and the film subject are coplanar. The likelihood exists
that there are out-of-plane errors in Glickman's calculations.
Again under ideal laboratory conditions, a subject (179 cm tall) and a
calibration standard (Daegling's standing height) were filmed with the
lens perpendicular to the reference plane 80 feet (2,438 cm) away [ILLUSTRATION
FOR FIGURE 2 OMITTED], a distance comparable to estimates of the distance
of the film subject from the camera at the onset of what is regarded as
the best sequence of footage. When positioned in the reference plane with
the subject, the standard (Daegling) provides an excellent estimate of
subject height (178.4 cm, an error below 1 percent). When the standard
is out of plane by only 1 m (closer to lens) the subject's apparent height
is 172.3 cm, nearly 4 percent less than the true stature. As the standard
moves even closer to the camera and increasingly out of plane, the error
is exacerbated; true stature is underestimated by nearly 15 percent (152.4
cm) at a position 4 m out of plane.(9)
Table 1. Striding and Compliant gaitsSubject Gait type
speed (m/sec) stride length (cm)Daegling striding
2.1 (0.18) 216 (7.6)
compliant 3.1 (0.15) 293 (10.7)
Schmitt striding 2.2 (0.34) 208 (15.9)
compliant 3.0 (0.08) 288 (6.5)
Subject #3 striding 2.5 (0.59) 224 (53.2)
compliant 3.8 (0.31) 338 (22.8)
Subject#4 striding 2.2 (0.14) 201 (13.1)
compliant 2.9 (0.10) 269 (9.7)
film subject compliant 16 fps 1.9 237-310
18 fps 2.1
24 fps 2.8Means and standard
deviations are given for N = 4 (Daegling & Schmitt), N = 6 (subject
#3) and N = 10 (subject #4) trials. Statures are 194.5 cm (Daegling),
188 cm (Schmitt), 176 cm (subject
#3, an adult male), 183 cm (subject #4, another adult male), and 222 cm
(film subject, Glickman's estimate). Speeds and stride lengths are calculated
from forced, high-speed walks in human subjects.
Stride for the film subject is given as a range of estimates (Perez 1992;
Glickman 1998). Film speed is not known, so three estimates for subject
velocity are given for different film speeds (D.W.
Grieve, reprinted in Byrne 1975). Claims that the film subject walks too
fast for a human are countered by the observation that compliant gaits
can increase walking speed to match or exceed that of the film subject.
These errors assume that the lens is positioned perpendicular to the reference
plane, to satisfy the need for calibration object and subject to be positioned
equidistant from the optical axis of the camera lens. The various sketches
reconstructing camera and subject position in the Patterson-Gimlin film,
however, suggest that the camera was only intermittently, if ever, fully
perpendicular to the reference plane.(10) This would result in dissimilar
distances of calibration object and film subject from the camera lens,
with attendant errors in stature estimation. How serious is this problem?
When we rotated the camera 5 degrees off an intended reference plane [ILLUSTRATION
FOR FIGURE 3 OMITTED], a calibration standard of 176 cm failed to predict
Schmitt's stature of 188 cm with acceptable accuracy, even though scalar
and subject were "coplanar."
The apparent stature was 172 cm, fully 8 percent smaller than true stature.(11)
Glickman suggests that the error of his estimate of the film subject may
be on the order of one inch ([less than] 3 cm) although no error analysis
is provided to verify this. If this error magnitude is to be accepted,
the following conditions must have been met: (1) the lens was positioned
perpendicular to the reference plane in which measurements were made,
(2) the subject and calibration object were both positioned in the plane
of reference such that they are equidistant from the optical axis of the
lens, and (3) the scalar dimension measured on the calibration object
was precisely in line with the reference plane (i.e., not off-angle relative
the camera lens). Published material to date does not demonstrate that
these conditions apply.
Glickman's estimates of other subject parameters derived from linear measurements
(specifically, chest circumference and body weight) will have errors compounded
in the process of calculation. He bases his estimate of chest circumference
on a formula for an ellipse that uses linear estimates of chest depth
and width as minor and major axes. To reconstruct these axes, Glickman
attempts to correct for out-of-plane rotation of the subject's chest.
Given the uncertainty of subject position, camera position, and the lack
of an accurate independent scalar in the film, it is virtually inconceivable
that a mathematical correction would yield a true estimate (see Miller
and Petak 1973; Noss 1967; Schmitt 1994; Chan 1997).
Glickman uses an allometric equation based on living primates to calculate
a body mass for the film subject of 1,957 lbs. - absurdly large unless
one wishes to posit that Bigfoot is constructed of nonstandard biological
tissues.(12) Krantz's estimate of the film subject's stature is 6[feet]6[inches]
(198 cm), well within human limits, but he argues that the chest width
of the subject is incompatible with the human form: "I can confidently
state that no man of that stature is built that broadly."(13) Assuming
that these parameters are measured without error, this assertion may be
refuted by a quick consultation of the Anthropometric Source Book (1978).
Chest width is measured by Krantz in the same fashion as a distance known
as "interscye" in the anthropometric literature. In a sample
of 1,004 men of the German Air Force, interscye of the ninety-fifth percentile
is 49.6 cm, a good 3 cm larger than Bigfoot's impossibly wide thorax.
The ninety-fifth percentile stature is 187.1 cm in this group, less than
4 inches shorter than the film subject. Unless Krantz would argue that
taller Air Force personnel necessarily have narrower chests, his confident
statement is admirable for its faith but not its veracity.
Nonhuman Locomotion?
Bigfoot proponents have suggested that the high-velocity, flexed-hip,
flexed-knee gait of the subject, which also walks with its trunk pitched
slightly forward, is absolutely nontypical of human locomotion. Krantz
again opines, "[J]udging from the way it walks, there is no possibility
that the film subject can be a man in a fur suit."(14) Repeated viewings
of the film suggest that, indeed, the subject does not exhibit the normal
striding gait of human bipedalism. But humans are capable of locomotion
that involves deeper flexion of the knees and hips similar to that seen
in the film subject, and this type of locomotion (a "compliant gait," Alexander 1992) has been studied under controlled conditions (Yaguramaki
et al. 1995; Li et al. 1996; Schmitt et al. 1996; Cook et al. 1997). Two
features of the subject's gait that Krantz asserts are atypical of humans
are in fact observable in humans who utilize a compliant gait: (1) reduction
of vertical oscillations of the head and trunk typical of striding bipedalism,
and (2) an extended period of support (weight-bearing) phase during a
locomotor cycle (McMahon et al. 1987; Alexander 1992; Schmitt et al. 1996).
Bigfoot investigators have also remarked upon the apparent high speed
of the film subject's gait and the length of its stride as being beyond
human capability.(15) When a compliant gait is employed, however, there
are two measurable effects: It is possible to walk faster, and the stride
length is increased (Table 1). Glickman's calculated stride length for
the film subject is 237 cm. This figure is easily surpassed by the authors
and two other measured subjects, all of whom are less than 200 cm tall.
Reported stride lengths based on footprints at the film site range from
284 cm to 310 cm (Perez 1992). A human over 200 cm tall could be expected
to match or exceed these higher figures using a compliant gait.
Another peculiarity of the film subject is the forward pitch of the trunk
during locomotion. This is again atypical of striding bipedalism, but
when humans do adopt this strange posture, a compliant gait becomes the
obligate form of bipedalism (Yaguramaki et al. 1995). Assertions that
the kinematics of the film subject cannot be duplicated by human agents
are thus demonstrably false.
There remains the remarkable coincidence that independent studies have
yielded identical results for stature of the film subject. Does this not "validate" the results in the tradition of rigorous science?
Perhaps it would if Murphy's study did not involve a fudge factor in the
Form of a "stoop correction" that can be arbitrarily applied
to the stature estimate.(16) This observation, in addition to the enormous
potential for calibration errors in Murphy's study, suggests that the
convergent results are most charitably regarded as a happy coincidence.
Conclusion
Bigfoot proponents have long decried the undeniable fact that detailed
scientific investigation of the Patterson-Gimlin film has been lacking.
There are, however, intractable difficulties involved in obtaining reliable
data from image analysis where conditions are far from ideal. The potential
for calibration errors in analyses of the Patterson-Gimlin film has undermined
attempts to extract quantitative data from it.
Uncertainties in subject and camera positions doom quantitative analysis
of this centerpiece of Bigfoot lore, as do the additional problems posed
by poor image quality and artifacts of subject and camera movement. Just
as measurement of film parameters and qualitative assessment of kinematics
cannot demonstrate that the film subject is nonhuman, there are probably
no means by which to demonstrate conclusively that the film subject represents
a human agent either. based on our analysis of gait and problems inherent
in estimating subject dimensions, it is our opinion that it is not possible
to evaluate the identity of the film subject with any confidence. Consequently,
the Patterson-Gimlin film does not provide unequivocal proof of Bigfoot's
existence, nor can the film be used to substantiate other putative images
of Bigfoot (e.g. the Redwoods "playmate" video).
Acknowledgments
We owe a great debt to many individuals who provided invaluable information
regarding the film and the circumstances surrounding it. First and foremost,
Rene Dahinden has unfailingly given his time to provide background on
the film. Michael Dennett is also thanked for his efforts in obtaining
various bits of obscure but critical detail. Peter Byrne of the now-defunct
Bigfoot Research Project graciously provided assistance in the early phases
of this investigation. Andrew Trueblood assisted with data acquisition
and analysis. Marnie Wiss provided editorial expertise and substantive
criticism of earlier drafts.
Notes
1. Progressive Research is a British Columbia-based organization
investigating Bigfoot evidence and marketing Bigfoot merchandise. Murphy's
research results are posted on the Internet Virtual Big foot Conference
(IVBC) established by Henry Franzoni. Readers may access the conference
at www.teleport.com/~caveman/ivbc.html. These results are also summarized
in NASI News (Vol. I).
2. Glickman is a certified forensic examiner who specializes in
image enhancement and reconstruction. He has served as the executive director
of the North American Science Institute (NASI), an organization dedicated
to getting to the bottom of the Bigfoot phenomenon. His research results
appear in "Toward a Resolution of the Bigfoot Phenomenon," a
1998 publication of the institute.
3. Chris Murphy's IVBC posting of June 9, 1997, states, "With
the current findings, there is little room for doubt on the reality of
the creature in the Patterson-Gimlin film. In other words, it was NOT
a hoax." Similarly incautious conclusions can be found peppered throughout
the various Web sites dealing with Bigfoot.
4. Rene Dahinden, personal communication, July 15, 1998. Dahinden
has been involved in Bigfoot research for over forty years, and has been
investigating the circumstances of the Patterson film since it was made
in 1967.
5. John Green mapped the site in June 1968, Reno Dahinden did so
in 1971, and Peter Byrne undertook the task in 1972. Grover Krantz used
Titmus's schematic and his own analysis of the film to produce a revised
diagram. The various schematics are in general agreement but differ in
details of the exact path of the film subject and its relation to the
camera.
6. Of course, subject and object may always be said to be coplanar,
except that registration of two points does not define a unique plane.
To establish that objects are coplanar requires registration of at least
three points, or a line and a point. In the case of the Patterson film,
the desired reference plane corresponds to the vertical extension of the "line" of travel of the film subject.
7. Images were digitized using Peak Performance software, Motus
4.0. Englewood, Colorado. To eliminate any of our own bias in assessing
calibration errors, we used a "blind" measurement protocol.
Five individuals, who were not informed of study objectives were asked
to measure calibration standards. Each individual was then asked to measure
subject stature for each test in a randomized design (e.g., individual
I would measure stature based on individual 3's calibration, and no individual
had knowledge of true values of calibration standards or subjects). Values
reported in the text represent the mean value over five observations.
8. While the calibration object is of preferred size, the fact
that both subject and this object are small relative to the viewing volume
is less than ideal for quantitative analysis.
9. Subject height will be similarly overestimated if the standard
is positioned behind the subject. Ironically, as the distance of subject
and standard from the lens increases, the errors will be smaller for a
given distance that they are off-plane, but they will still exist. At
ever-increasing distances, however, image quality may deteriorate. This
may be especially true in cases where objects are small within a film
frame, as occurs throughout the Patterson-Gimlin film.
10. Schematic drawings by Krantz, Titmus, Dahinden, and Green are
reproduced in Bigfoot at Bluff Creek by Danny Perez, 1992, Center for
Bigfoot Studies, Norwalk, California.
11. With respect to conditions of the Patterson-Gimlin film, the
magnitude of this source of error depends on the interactive effects of
(1) the degree of obliquity of the lens' optical axis relative to the
intended reference plane, (2) the distance of subject and standard from
each other, and (3) the distance of each from the camera lens.
12. Assuming the film subject has a density similar to water (reasonable
for most animals), one can model the subject as a water-filled cylinder
and mathematically "weigh" it, with no allowance for tapering
of the head or lower limbs. This procedure produces a very rough high-end
estimate of mass. Assuming Glickman's estimates are accurate, this cylinder
weighs 1,625 lbs., well below the 1,957 lb. figure given in the NASI report.
The nearly one-ton Bigfoot weighs nearly five times more than a large
male gorilla and outweighs tire heaviest polar bears by over 200kg (McDonald
1984).
13. Page 118, Big Footprints. It is unclear whether "that
stature" to which Krantz refers is his own or the film subject's.
For the subsequent argument, it scarcely matters.
14. Page 115, Big Footprints.
15. Rene Dahinden (personal communication and Perez 1992) reports
that, on one of his numerous visits to the film site, he and others present
were incapable of walking the distance traveled by the film subject in
the time that it did so (assuming a film speed of 24 fps).
16. Calculation of a "stoop correction" is unnecessary
in any case: one can measure body segments regardless of their orientation
to one another and sum these to arrive at stature.
References
Alexander, R.M. 1992. A model of bipedal locomotion on compliant legs.
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, London. B338: 189-198.
Anthropometric Source Book. 1978. NASA Reference Publication 1024. U.S.
Department of Commerce, National Technical Information Service.
Byrne, P. 1975. The Search for Bigfoot: Monster, Myth, or Man. Washington,
D.C.: Acropolis Books.
Chan, L-K. 1997. Thoracic shape and shoulder biomechanics in primates.
Ph.D. dissertation, Duke University.
Cook, T.M., K.P. Farrell, I.A. Carey, J.M. Gibbs, and G.E. Wiger. 1997.
Effects of restricted knee flexion and walking speed on the vertical ground
reaction force. Journal of Orthopaedics and Sports Physical Therapy 25:
236-244.
Glickman, J. 1998, Toward a Resolution of the Bigfoot Phenomenon. Photek
Research Report. Hood River: North American Science Institute.
Green, J. 1981. Sasquatch: The Apes Among Us. Seattle: Hancock House.
Krantz, G. 1992. Big Footprints. Boulder, Colorado: Johnson Books.
Li, Y., R.H. Cromptom, R.M. Alexander, M.M. Gunther, and W.J. Wang. 1996.
Characteristics of ground reaction forces in normal and chimpanzee-like
bipedal walking by humans. Folia Primatologica 66: 137-159.
McDonald, D. 1984. The Encyclopedia of Mammals. New York: Facts on File.
McMahon, T.A., G. Valiant, and E.C. Frederick. 1987. Groucho running.
Journal of Applied Physiology 62: 2326-2337.
Miller, D., and K. Petak. 1973. Three dimensional cinematography. Kinesiology
14-17.
Noss, J. 1967. Control of photographic perspective in motion analysis.
Journal of Health, Physical Education and Recreation 38: 81-84.
Perez, D. 1992. Bigfoot at Bluff Creek. Norwalk: Center for BigFoot Studies.
Schmitt, D. 1994. Forelimb mechanics as a function of substrate type during
quadrupedalism in two anthropoid primates. Journal of Human Evolution
26: 441-457.
Schmitt, D.O., J.T. Stern, Jr., and S.G. Larson. 1996. Compliant gait
in humans: Implications for substrate reaction forces during australopithecine
bipedalism. American Journal of Physical Anthropology Suppl. 22: 209.
Yaguramaki, N., S. Nishizawa, K. Adachi, and B. Endo. 1995. The relationship
between posture and external force in walking. Anthropological Sciences
103: 117-140.
About the author:
Dr. David Daegling, Ph.D., was formerly a professor of anthropology at
Yale University, is now in the Anthropology Department at the University
of Florida. He has a new book about to be released in the fall of 2004
titled "Bigfoot Exposed : An Anthropologist Examines America's
Enduring Legend" (University of Florida) -- AltaMira Press $72.00.
Fall 1985; B.A. 1982, Pitzer College; M.A. 1989, SUNY Stony Brook; Ph.D.
1990, Dr. Frederick Grine; Dissertation: Geometry and Biomechanics of
Hominoid Mandibles; Placement or Plans: Asst. Professor, Dept. of Anthropology,
Yale University, New Haven, CT.
Dr. Daniel Schmitt Ph.D., is a professor in the Department of Biological
Anthropology and Anatomy and heads the Vertebrate Movement Laboratory
at Duke University Medical Center. Fall 1988; B.S. 1988, Yale University;
M.A. 1991, SUNY, Stony Brook Ph.D. 1995, Dr. Susan Larson
Dissertation: A Kinematic and Kinetic Analysis of Forelimb Use During
Arboreal and Terrestrial Quadrupedalism in Old World Monkeys Placement
or Plans: Res. Assoc., Dept. of Biological Anthropology and Anatomy, Duke
University, Durham, NC.
COPYRIGHT 1999 Committee for the Scientific Investigation of Claims of
the Paranormal
COPYRIGHT 2000 Gale Group
- ---
Back
to What's New?
Back
to Newspaper & Magazine Articles
Home/Main
Portions of
this website are reprinted and sometimes edited to fit the standards of
this website under the Fair Use Doctrine of International Copyright Law
as educational material without benefit of financial gain.
http://www4.law.cornell.edu/uscode/17/107.html
This proviso is applicable
throughout the entire Bigfoot
Encounters
Website.
|